Автор: Пользователь скрыл имя, 07 Апреля 2012 в 20:48, курсовая работа
У процесі розвитку таких різних галузей, як медицина та переклад все частіше стає актуальним питання їх поєднання – перекладу медичних текстів. В наш час все частіше медичні тексти присвячені питанням генетики, дослідженням генетики, виконаним на різних мовах. В дослідженні перекладу текстів у галузі генетики і полягає актуальність даної роботи. Новизна полягає у дослідженні саме використання трансформацій при перекладі текстів у галузі генетики.
ВСТУП
РОЗДІЛ 1. ПЕРЕКЛАДАЦЬКІ ТРАНСФОРМАЦІЇ ЯК СПОСІБ АДЕКВАТНОГО ПЕРЕКЛАДУ
1.1. Поняття адекватності перекладу
1.2. Перекладацькі трансформації як частина функціонального способу перекладу
1.2.1. Лексичні трансформації
1.2.2. Граматичні трансформації
1.2.3. Лексико-семантичні трансформації
РОЗДІЛ 2. ТЕКСТ ПЕРЕКЛАДУ
РОЗДІЛ 3. ВИКОРИСТАННЯ ТРАНСФОРМАЦІЙ ПРИ ПЕРЕКЛАДІ АНГЛОМОВНИХ ТЕКСТІВ У ГАЛУЗІ ГЕНЕТИКИ
3.1. Лексичні трансформації
3.2. Граматичні трансформації
3.3. Лексико-семантичні трансформації
ВИСНОВКИ
СПИСОК ВИКОРИСТАНОЇ ЛІТЕРАТУРИ
ДОДАТОК А
ДОДАТОК Б
17. Раєвська Н. М. English lexicology - Київ 1991 р. – 234 c.
18. РецкерЯ.И. Теория перевода й переводческая практика. -М.: Междунар.отношения, 1974. – 228 c.
19. Селшанова О. О. Актуальні напрями сучасної лінгвістики — К.: Вища школа, 1999. – 324 с.
20. Словарь-справочник лингвистических терминов. Изд. 2-е. — М.: Просвещение. Розенталь Д. Э., Теленкова М. А.. 1976.
21. Хидекель С.С., Трудности английского словоупотребления / С.С.Хидекель, М.Р.Кауль, Е.Л.Гинзбург. - М.: Академия, 2005. – 158c.
22. Швейцер А.Д. Очерк современного английского языка в США / Александр Давидович. - М., Международные отношения, 1993. – 217c.
23. Швейцер А.Д. Английский язык / А.Д.Швейцер, В.Н.Ярцева. - М.: АСТ, 2004. – 288c.
24. Шевченко Й. С. Лінгвістичні й дидактичні проблеми іншомовної комунікації. - Харків: Константа, 2002p. – 284 c.
25. Bassnett-McGuire, S. Translation studies. London & New York: Methuem. 1980. – 178 p.
26. Brislin, R. W. Translation: application and research. New York: Gardner Press Inc. 1976. – 228 p.
27. Catford, J.C. A linguistic theory of translation. London: Oxford University Press. 1984. – 259 p.
28. Lewis M. M. Language in Society. London: Nelson 1978. 192 p.
29. Newmark, P. About Translation: Multilingual Matters. Clevedon, Philadelphia, Adelaide: Multilingual Matters Ltd. 1991. 342 p.
30. Nida, E. A. Language, structure, and translation: essays by Nida. Stanford: Stanford University Press. 1975. 218 p.
31. Reiss, K. Text types, translation types and translation assessment. In Chesterman, A. (Ed.), Readings in translation theory (pp. 105-15). Helsinki: Oy Finn Lectura Ab. 1989. 286 p.
32. Savory, T. The art of translation. London: Jonathan Cape Ltd. 1979. 246 p.
ДОДАТОК А
Programmed for speech
Scientific American Mind
By Sebastian Haesler
Does speech—that uniquely human trait—come from our genes, or is it learned? Luminaries such as linguist Noam Chomsky of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology have championed the role of evolutionary inheritance over that of culture. But for many years, proponents of this position could only look to languages themselves for evidence. They observed that many tongues share grammatical structures and other attributes, bolstering the argument that speech is innate. The suspicion that a “speech gene” might exist, however, remained unresolved. Then, in 1990, something extraordinary happened.
It could not have been a coincidence that a particularly large number of children from one family showed up at an English speech therapy school. The children mumbled almost unintelligibly and stumbled over gram-mar —they could not, for instance, describe events in a correct chronological order.
In studies led by Jane A. Hurst at Oxford Radcliffe Hospital in England, researchers found that the affected members of the family, dubbed the KEs, had a physically normal speech apparatus—lips, jaw, tongue and vocal chords. Their other fine-motor skills were normal, as were their hearing and IQ. For three generations about half the family members had suffered from the same speech defect. Clearly, the disorder had a genetic component and was specific enough in its effects to offer the hope that it was directly connected to that elusive speech gene.
When the gene responsible for the impediment was pinpointed just a few years later, it finally provided evidence that the ability to speak is indeed written in our DNA. But how exactly do genes regulate a complicated mental process such as speech? Studies of the gene in people and in our animal cousins — especially songbirds, whose vocal learning resembles that of people - could help explain why speech evolved in humans but not in any other species. They might also lead to therapies for speech impediments like the one plaguing the KE family.
Language in Our Genes
Geneticists led by Simon E. Fisher of the University of Oxford identified in the KE family a segment on chromosome 7 in which there must have been a mutation. But determining which of the dozens of genes in that segment was at fault promised to be a lengthy process of trial and error. The researchers got a lucky break when they found a boy from an unrelated family who had a similar speech problem. The boy had a visible defect on chromosome 7 in the same segment as the one that looked suspicious in the KE family. The chromosome was broken at a gene known as FOXP2, so the researchers started looking specifically at that gene. In 2001 they successfully identified FOXP2 mutations in KE family members with the speech defect, and the same defect was later confirmed in other people with similar speech impediments.
Ever since the first published reports about FOXP2, molecular geneticists and linguists have been engaged in a vociferous debate about how, precisely, the gene affects speech. Although the gene appears to be crucial for normal development, its specific role remains to be clarified. FOXP2 codes for a protein that affects hundreds or perhaps even thousands of other genes, and scientists have barely begun to understand its complex influence. Mutations in the FOXP2 gene appear to hinder the development of brain regions responsible for motor control as well as regions involved in language processing. Furthermore, the FOXP2 gene exists in a variety of species—from reptiles to mammals—so it must serve other functions besides facilitating speech.
Many researchers, including my team at the Max Planck Institute for Molecular Genetics in Berlin, are especially interested in FOXP2 in birds, because some songbirds learn their songs in a way that is strikingly similar to how children learn speech. By studying the role of FOXP2 in birdsong, we are revealing how it might affect the development of language in people.
Genes ensure that the brain develops normally in a number of ways. Specialized nerve cells need to be formed, they need to produce the correct connections to neighbouring cells, and they need to be able to emit signals or conduct messages to other neurons. In addition, nerve cells in the brain must develop the capacity to process information so that they can “learn” things. Gene products, namely, proteins into which the genetic code is translated, are involved in all these processes.
FOXP2 codes for a transcription factor, a protein that binds to other segments of DNA, thereby affecting whether or not different genes are read and translated into their respective gene products. (“FOX” stands for “forkhead box” [see box above], which refers to the specific DNA sequence that codes for the part of the protein that latches onto other DNA molecules.)
As a transcription factor, the FOXP2 protein serves as an on-off switch for numerous target genes. Because all genetic material exists in duplicate (except in the case of the male Y chromosome), a FOXP2 mutation on one chromosome causes the body to produce only half as much of the transcription factor as it should. The resulting shortage somehow causes the speech defect found in the KE family.
Complex Interactions
To find out how the disturbed regulation of FOXP2 genes leads to speech disorders, we must first identify the regions of the brain in which FOXP2 is normally active. We can draw conclusions about the function of a particular gene based on when and where it is expressed, meaning when and where the cell produces a protein in accordance with the DNA blueprint.
The FOXP2 protein is produced very early on in the embryo’s developing brain, particularly in those regions that later become the cerebellum, the thalamus and the basal ganglia. Consistent with this pattern of expression, a structural analysis of the brains of patients with the telltale speech defect revealed that the volumes of their cerebellum and basal ganglia differed from those of people with unimpaired speech. In addition, when these patients spoke, parts of their basal ganglia were less active relative to those of normal subjects.
Both the basal ganglia and cerebellum control body movements. They are activated whenever complex motor skills are learned, such as those involved in playing a piano. Presumably these regions are also responsible for motor function during the formation of sounds. It seems plausible that the KE family’s difficulty in articulating words is rooted in the malformation of these areas of the brain.
The effects of a faulty FOXP2 gene do not stop there, however. Brain changes also appear in two well-known cortical speech centers: the Wernicke speech area and Broca’s region. Neurolinguists have long suspected that Wernicke’s area controls the understanding of speech, whereas Broca’s region is involved in the production of speech. We know now, however, that this strict division is a little too cut-and-dried because a number of other areas of the brain are involved in both understanding and producing speech. The human brain probably processes spoken information in many areas of the brain simultaneously.
The idea that the brain uses parallel processing to understand and control speech is consistent with another observation: in the KEs, parts of the brain that are normally not involved in speech are active. This activity could be a direct consequence of the FOXP2 defect—a proper amount of the transcription factor would have made those areas behave normally — or it could be a sign that the brain is attempting to compensate for one of the other flaws caused by the mutation.
Differentiating between the direct and indirect effects of FOXP2 is no simple matter. For example, because the gene is active during the embryonic stage, its dysfunction could disturb brain development. The brain could be “wired” incorrectly, or certain specialized nerve cells could fail to form. On the opposite end of the spectrum of possibility, the brain might develop normally but run into problems with information processing later on — for example, during the phase when children learn to speak. The real effects of the FOXP2 mutation probably lie somewhere in between these two extremes.
Bird Babble
To probe further FOXP2’s effects on cognitive development, researchers are turning to animals for clues. The FOXP2 gene has been identified in primates, whales, birds and even crocodiles; it is highly likely that all vertebrates have it. The sequence of the gene in these animals is almost identical to that in humans. For example, only three of the 715 amino acids in the mouse FOXP2 gene product differ from those in the human version. The timing and location of the gene’s expression in the brains of other species are also very similar. So what is the FOXP2 gene doing in the brains of these animals, none of which is capable of speech?
Although most animals have vocalizations that seem to be innate, a few species - among them songbirds, parrots, hummingbirds, some marine mammals and bats - do learn vocal patterns by imitating their parents. To some extent, this process is similar to that of a human infant making his or her first efforts at learning speech. At first, baby sparrows can imitate only minute elements of their future song, for instance. This type of vocalization is referred to as subsong, and it is similar to infant babbling. When the young animal hears an example of what is correct, it adapts its vocal output.
Through intensive practice, young songbirds increasingly come to sound like their role models, mastering the repertoire by the onset of sexual maturity. As is the case with humans, songbirds are dependent on what they hear to develop normal vocalization. If songbirds are subjected to loud noises, if they become deaf or if the feedback from their “teacher” is interrupted, they never learn to sing properly.
The similarities between learned avian song and human speech run even deeper. Both humans and songbirds have developed neuronal structures that specialize in the perception and production of sounds. Compared with humans, whose brains use parallel processing to comprehend speech, songbirds have a rather more modularly constructed brain in which various centers assume specific roles. In the avian brain, auditory stimuli reach the high vocal center, which controls the muscular movements of the vocal organ via the motor center; damage to this region prevents singing.
Another important data path in bird brains extends from the high vocal center via area X —a song-learning center in the basal ganglia —to the thalamus and from there back to the cortex. This so-called corticobasal ganglia loop also exists in the brains of mammals, including humans, where it is vital for learning. In young birds, lesions in area X lead to abnormal twittering, whereas such lesions seem to have no effect in the adults of most songbird species — until they try to learn a new song. Apparently the corticobasal network is important for learning songs but not necessarily for singing them. In humans also, FOXP2 proteins are produced in large quantities in the basal ganglia, which is where the structural and functional anomalies occur in patients with FOXP2-related speech defects.
Genetic Songwriter
In the brains of zebra finches, area X contains more FOXP2 during the song-learning phase than during infancy or adulthood. Likewise in the canary, which changes its melody once a year after breeding season, FOXP2 is expressed particularly strongly in area X during this learning phase.
Accordingly, FOXP2 may well be involved in song plasticity —the ability to learn new songs. To explore this possibility, our team genetically reduced the amount of FOXP2 in area X in zebra finches to artificially induce a situation similar to a FOXP2 mutation in humans. The crucial question is, What happens to a melody if less FOXP2 is expressed in area X while it is being learned?
Our initial results show that the zebra finches have difficulty learning their songs when they have less than the normal amount of the FOXP2 transcription factor. We concluded that this protein is necessary for zebra finches to learn a song, but it is less important for motor functions overall. Thus, a FOXP2 mutation does not simply cause the brain to develop abnormally. The defect continues to play a role once the brain has become fully developed —an important clue about what happens in humans who have the speech problems displayed by the KE family.
Careful analogy of the zebra finch’s impaired song learning with the problems seen in the KE family leads us to believe that affected family members may have difficulty imitating the sounds made by their parents. They are unable to harmonize their own speech with that of others. Should this suspicion be borne out, it would mean that the similarity between avian song learning and human speech acquisition extends all the way to the molecular level.
The logical implication is that the evolution of language is not a unique feature of the human lineage. Many species share the structure and molecular makeup of the brain that was already in place as our ancestors began to speak. Only as existing genes and neuronal systems continued to develop was the path cleared for the uniquely human capacity for speech.
ДОДАТОК Б
Children With Klinefelter Syndrome
Arthur Schoenstadt, MD
Children with Klinefelter syndrome are very similar to other children their age. The most significant differences between children with Klinefelter syndrome and children without it are delays in language acquisition and development. Children with Klinefelter syndrome have the greatest difficulty with expressive language, which is the ability to put thoughts, ideas, and emotions into words.
An Overview of Children With Klinefelter Syndrome
Children with Klinefelter syndrome are not much different from other children their age. However, children with Klinefelter syndrome usually learn to walk later than most other children, and may have similar delays in learning to speak.
Language delays in children with Klinefelter syndrome are particularly important for parents to pay attention to. Klinefelter syndrome research has shown that seeking help early for language delays will greatly reduce any associated long-term effects.
Temperament of Children With Klinefelter Syndrome
Children with Klinefelter syndrome tend to start life as what many parents call "good babies" -- quiet, undemanding, and perhaps even a little passive. As toddlers, they may be somewhat shy and reserved. Throughout childhood -- perhaps, even, for the rest of their lives -- children with Klinefelter syndrome retain the same temperament and disposition they first displayed as infants and toddlers.
As a group, they tend to be shy, somewhat passive, and unlikely to take a leadership role. Although they do make friends with other children, they tend to have only a few friends at a time. Klinefelter research scientists also describe them as cooperative and eager to please.
Language Delays in Children With Klinefelter Syndrome Most males with Klinefelter syndrome have some degree of language impairment. As children, they often learn to speak much later than other children do, and they may have difficulty learning to read and write. And while they eventually do learn to speak and converse normally, the majority of males with Klinefelter syndrome tend to have some degree of difficulty with language throughout their lives. If untreated, this language impairment can lead to poor performance in school and its attendant loss of self-esteem.
In some children with Klinefelter syndrome, the language delays may be more severe, with the child not fully learning to talk until about age 5. Others may learn to speak at a normal rate, and not meet with any problems until they begin school, where they may experience reading difficulties. A few may not have any problems at all with learning to speak or learning to read.
Children with Klinefelter syndrome usually have difficulty with expressive language, which is the ability to put thoughts, ideas, and emotions into words. In contrast, their faculty for receptive language -- understanding what is said -- is close to normal.
Social Skills in Children With Klinefelter Syndrome
In addition to academic help, children with Klinefelter syndrome, like other language-disabled children, may need help with social skills. Language is essential, not only for learning the school curriculum, but also for building social relationships. By talking and listening, children make friends, and in the process, share information, attitudes, and beliefs. Through language, they also learn how to behave, not just in the schoolroom, but also on the playground. If a child's language disability seems to prevent him from fitting in socially, the parents of a child with Klinefelter syndrome may want to ask school officials about a social skills training program.
Seeking Help Early for Children With Klinefelter Syndrome
The parents of children with Klinefelter syndrome can compensate for their child's language disability by providing special help in language development, beginning at an early age. However, there is no easy formula to meet the language needs of all children with Klinefelter syndrome. Like everyone else, children with Klinefelter syndrome are unique individuals. A few may not have any trouble learning to read and write, while the rest may have language impairments ranging from mild to severe.
If their son's speech seems to be lagging behind that of other children, parents should ask their child's pediatrician for a referral to a speech pathologist for further testing. A speech pathologist specializes in the disorders of voice, speech, and language.
Public Law 99-457 is an amendment to Public Law 94-142 that assists states in providing special education services for infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. Eligibility requirements and entrance procedures vary from state to state. Parents can call the Federation for Children with Special Needs to learn which agencies to contact in their area.
Parents should also pay particular attention to their child's hearing. Like other small children, infants and toddlers with Klinefelter syndrome may suffer from frequent ear infections. With any child, such infections may impair hearing and delay the acquisition of language. Such a hearing impairment may be a further setback for children with Klinefelter syndrome who are already having language difficulties.
Информация о работе Використання трансформацій при перекладі англомовних текстів у галузі генетики